boy with blocks

This module is a innovative professional development resource for busy early childhood educators. It provides teachers with some suggested ways to help children meet the following benchmark…

Mathematics
10.B.ECb: Make predictions about the outcome prior to collecting information, with teacher support and multiple experiences over time.

Performance Descriptors

IELDS benchmark performance descriptors give a general sense of what a child might be able to do related to this benchmark if

  • her experience is at the beginning level (exploring),
  • she has some experience and has begun to construct her understanding (developing), or
  • her understanding is more solid (building).

The performance descriptors for IELDS Benchmark 10.B.ECb are:

EXPLORINGDEVELOPINGBUILDING
With teacher support, begin to predict the outcome of an activity (e.g., predict there are more boys than girls at the snack table).With teacher support, provide a reasonable prediction or guess for the outcome of an activity
(e.g., predict that the class collected more yellow than red leaves on
the nature walk before sorting and counting them).
With teacher support, predict with more accuracy the outcome of a counting or comparison activity
(e.g., predict how many more chairs, when three are already there, are needed for the small group table so that six children can all have a seat).

Explanation

When we say that we want children to be able to “make predictions about the outcome prior to collecting information, with teacher support and multiple experiences over time” —what does that mean?

Let’s Break It Down

What do these terms mean?

Prediction

In this benchmark, prediction refers to imagining possible answers to a question about something that has not happened yet. A prediction is a guess about what might happen. Prediction is very important in math and science, but people make predictions in all areas of life. In fact, we make predictions every day, such as “If I say those words to my friend, she might feel sad” or “We’re supposed to get snow, so I should wear boots.”

Prediction involves what is sometimes called foresight: the ability to make a more-or-less logical guess about what comes next in a sequence, or about the outcome of an event or phenomenon. Some specialists have called this ability to look ahead “mental time travel.”

Prediction is related to a range of concepts and behaviors, including:

  • Probability: the likelihood that something will occur.
  • Expectation or anticipation: the feeling that something in particular will happen.
  • Sequencing: the order in which events occur or objects are organized according to a particular characteristic, such as length (longest to shortest).
  • Making a plan: creating a more-or-less detailed strategy, design, or scheme before beginning a task or activity.
  • Preparing: getting ready to do something or getting ready for something to happen; collecting materials, etc., for implementing a plan.

There are several ways of thinking about the kinds of predictions young children can make.

  • With a wild guess, a child takes a chance on making a prediction about something with which she has little experience or knowledge.
  • She might make a thoughtful or educated guess when her previous experience and knowledge enable her to make a prediction that seems reasonable under the circumstances.
  • After more experience, a child might develop a hypothesis about something—a proposed explanation of how or why something happens, which she can test by setting up an experiment. A hypothesis is the starting point for an investigation: “I think this will happen because…”  A hypothesis may or may not turn out to be “right.”

Research has shown that young children’s ability to “look ahead” is different from that of adults and older children. Studies have even found differences between the abilities of 3-year-olds and 5-year-olds to predict. Preschool teachers know that young children often have difficulty imagining the future.

When very young children explore their environments, tinker with materials, and try to solve problems through trial and error, they lay the groundwork for deeper understandings of what is possible and what is unlikely or impossible. They need time to play and to observe the world around them so they become aware of patterns, of “the usual order” of events, and of cause-and-effect relationships. They are likely to need time to make “wild guesses,” to check the outcome against what they guessed, and to have many similar experiences before they can make what adults consider a logical or reasonable prediction. They need opportunities to make predictions related to numbers and counting, patterns, stories, physical activities, changes in the world around them—all aspects of life. It also helps if they have thoughtful questioning and guidance from adults and more experienced peers to challenge or support these understandings and help them make increasingly accurate predictions.

Having time to explore the environment is important to a child’s developing ability to make predictions.

Information

In this benchmark, the word information means “what children find out about something.” Information is “facts” or data that can help children answer questions about an object or event—questions that begin with words such as what, when, where, who, how, and why. Information can include location, size, shape, texture, quantity, time, and other important facts and details about the object or event.

Children collect information when they observe an object or event and record their findings by sketching, taking notes, photographing, or making video. Collecting information is essential to helping children check their predictions against the findings of their inquiry so they can begin to see what accurate or reasonable prediction is like. Being able to compare their research findings with their guesses is an essential part of helping young children learn to make predictions.

Figure 1. A variety of experiences contribute to a child’s ability to make predictions
Figure 2. A child who has multiple experiences with something may be able to develop a hypothesis about it
Figure 3. Collecting information enables children to compare their predictions about a topic to what they find out about it

Scientists are studying the mental processes involved in making predictions, both for adults and for children. It seems likely that a child’s ability to make predictions co-develops with his self-regulation, memory, ability to create mental images, understandings of cause/effect, and his ability to plan and prepare, as well as the abilities and skills addressed in several other IELDS benchmarks, including:

Mathematics

  • 10.A.ECa: With teacher assistance, come up with meaningful questions that can be answered through gathering information.
  • 10.A.ECb: Gather data about themselves and their surroundings to answer meaningful questions
  • 10.B.ECa: Organize, represent, and analyze information using concrete objects, pictures, and graphs, with teacher support.
  • 10.C.ECa: Describe likelihood of events with appropriate vocabulary, such as “possible”, “impossible”, “always”, and “never”.

Science

  • 11.A.ECc: Plan and carry out simple investigations.
  • 11.A.ECd: Collect, describe, compare, and record information from observations and investigations.
  • 11.A.ECe: Use mathematical and computational thinking.
  • 11.A.ECf: Make meaning from experience and information by describing, talking, and thinking about what happened during an investigation.
  • 11.A.ECg: Generate explanations and communicate ideas and/or conclusions about their investigations.

Let’s Summarize

Benchmark 10.B.ECb says that we want children to be able to “make predictions about the outcome prior to collecting information, with teacher support and multiple experiences over time.” In other words, a child meets Benchmark 10.B.ECb if she can draw upon prior experiences to guess with some accuracy what might be the outcome of an activity or the next item/event in a sequence, with some guidance or help from the teacher or other adult.

Teacher’s Role

There are many ways for teachers to help preschool-age children address Benchmark 10.B.ECb. The following are some teacher-tested suggestions:

Planning

  • Explore teacher resources to find ways to engage children in making predictions throughout the day—math time, shared reading, project work, or outdoor play, while on a walk, etc.
  • Early in the year, think of ways you might create opportunities for children to make predictions during a variety of activities. Keep in mind that, depending on age and experience, your class may have a wide range of ability to do so.

Laying the Groundwork

  • Explain that a prediction is a guess about something that might happen but hasn’t happened yet. When you talk with the children, explain and use words such as certainly/probably, always/sometimes/never, possible/impossible.
  • Expect that children’s first predictions about anything may be “far from the mark.” For example, a 3-year-old may be unable to predict that something could happen “tomorrow” because her conception of time is very different from that of an older child or an adult. And a child who has little experience with number concepts might predict that he should put out 100 cups for snack, or 2 cups, for 20 children in the class.
  • If you notice children making predictions (perhaps without realizing it), talk with them about what they are doing and call it “predicting.” Let children join you in making some simple predictions, then checking on what actually happens. (See Predicting: Helping Preschoolers Look Ahead.)

Making Time for Predictions

  • Post a sign-in sheet with a new question every day, asking children to speculate about a variety of topics. Start with simple yes/no questions: Do you think it will snow today?
  • As children gain experience with predicting, pose sign-in questions that call for more complex reasoning: How many seeds do you predict we will find inside this apple? How many children do you think will be absent today?
  • Use a table to keep track of children’s questions, predictions, and findings during an investigation. (For more information, see The Question Table.)
QuestionsPredictionsFindings
Do you think trees have flowers on them?No, they only have leaves. (Sarah, Mick, Ella)

Yes. (Timaya)
Yes, many trees can have flowers. But the flowers don’t stay on all the time.
How many seeds are inside an apple?100 (Ka-Chuan)

17 (Roselyn, Kari)

8 (Franklin)
Yes, many trees can have flowers. But the flowers don’t stay on all the time.
  • From time to time, ask a child who is making a prediction, “What makes you think so?” Her response can give you insight into her thought processes. You might also see where additional experiences could help her.
  • With the children, look at the sign-in sheet or question/prediction table. Invite them to think about classmates’ predictions. “Ka-Chuan says he thinks the apple will have 100 seeds. Roselyn and Kari think it will have 17. Franklin, what do you think?” Call their attention to the range of predictions and whether several children made the same prediction.
  • Help children follow up on their predictions about a topic after they have collected data.
    • Invite children to compare what they predicted with what actually happened. (“The predictions of how many seeds in the apple were 100 seeds, 17 seeds, and 8 seeds. We made 10 tally marks when we counted the seeds, so that means 10 seeds were in that apple.”)
    • De-emphasize competition or “right” and “wrong.” Instead, focus on having children talk about how they arrived at their ideas. (“Ka-chuan, you’re saying you thought the apple would have 100 seeds because it looks like a pomegranate on the outside and pomegranates have lots of seeds. Then you noticed that the inside of an apple is not like a pomegranate at all.”)
  • Help children see that many predictions turn out to be incorrect. Children who are overly concerned about “being right” may not want to predict anything for fear their answers will be “wrong.” Several possible strategies can help a child in that position.
    • Make some wild guesses yourself and respond constructively when your predictions are wrong. Comments such as, “I was way off on that one!” or “My guess was pretty close” can show children how to deal with an “incorrect” guess.
    • Occasionally, try asking “What do you think WON’T happen?” This approach can free a child from the burden of feeling that she must always be right.
  • Include “educated guesses” in your conversations with children to show what it looks like to make an informed prediction: “I wonder if rain will fall here this morning. Hm, the sky is bright and sunny. Bright and sunny sky usually means it won’t rain soon. So I predict that rain won’t fall here and we can play outdoors.”
  • Play with predictions during transitions and waiting time.
    • Keep a list of simple prediction tasks handy for times when you and the children must wait for a turn to do something. Examples include: “0, 1, 2, 3. What do you think comes next?”  “I want to make a pattern: red bead/orange bead/red bead/orange bead/red bead. What color bead do I need next?”
    • Play “Close your eyes and guess.” For example, ask two or three children to “close your eyes and guess how many classmates are wearing boots.” After they guess, follow up by counting together to see how many children actually are wearing boots.
  • Ask children to make predictions about an experience they are about to have.
    • For example, before going outdoors, ask them to guess what they might see in the neighborhood or on the playground. “How many red cars do you think we will see during our walk?” “Who thinks we will hear an airplane flying overhead?” Make a list of their predictions and help them check off what they notice. (See Neighborhood Geography with Young Children.)
    • When preparing the class for a field trip, you might ask them to draw a picture of something they think they will see or hear. Revisit the drawings with them after the trip. Which predictions turned out to be accurate?
Figure 4. Children can start the day by answering a sign-in question.
Figure 5. A child may be overly concerned about making predictions that are “right”
Figure 6. Predicting the weather involves “educated guesses”

Using Predictions to Deepen Mathematical Understandings

  • Talk with children about sequences to help them become familiar with the concept of “what comes next.” (See Time for Preschoolers: In Sequence!)
    • Give them opportunities to link the counting sequence (1, 2, 3, 4, …) to quantities of objects.
    • Let them arrange objects according to length or height.
    • Provide opportunities for children to think about the order in which familiar events occur. For example, posting a visual schedule can help children see what to expect during their time in preschool, so they can begin to make predictions related to time.
  • Help children think about cause and effect.
    • Pose questions that help them consider what might make certain things happen. “When you put that cup of water outside, it turned to ice. The cup in here still has just water. If you put another cup of water outside, will it freeze, too?”
    • Encourage them to investigate cause and effect in their own ways. One way to do this is with “What if” questions. “Brendon wonders if he puts a cup of milk in the freezer all day, would it turn hard and cold? What do you think will happen?” (See Things Change or Playground Physics: Watch for Falling Objects.)
Figure 7. Posting a visual schedule can help children anticipate what to do next
pails
Figure 8. Children can learn predictable color patterns

Making Predictions Across the Curriculum

  • Involve children in planning for a variety of situations.
    • For example, ask a small group to think about what will be needed for snack: What will they need to dish up the cereal mix? How many cups and napkins should they set out so each child will have one? (See The Path to Math: More Numbers.)
    • Ask children to draw their plans for something they want to make. Help them list the materials they will need.
  • After children have had multiple experiences with something, suggest that they set up a related experiment. “What might happen if you use honey instead of sugar when you make muffins this morning?” “Imani thinks a cup of milk will be completely hard in 10 minutes in the freezer. How long do you think it will take to freeze?” (See Time for Preschoolers: Duration.)
  • Invite children to make predictions during shared reading. “You’ve looked at the illustration on the cover. What do you think might happen in the story?” “The giant just woke up, and he sees Jack running. What do you think he’ll do now?” When sharing books with rhyme patterns, let children have a chance to fill in words at the end of a line. (See “What’s Next?” Predictions at Story Time or Using Predictable Books with Young Children.)
  • Create “prediction challenges” for children on the playground or in the gym. “How many times do you think Ryan can go down the slide in one minute?” “Who do you think will be the second person to finish the race?”
  • Make active play a time for predicting. For example, when the class is getting ready to play Fox and Geese, ask, “How many geese do you think one fox can catch in one minute?” “How many minutes to catch all of the geese?” During target games, invite children to predict how many times they will hit the target during a turn or during the whole game.
Figure 9. Designing an experiment provides opportunities for a child to predict what might happen
Figure 10. Children can make predictions about what might happen next in a story

Documentation & Assessment

Documentation

Documentation can provide evidence of how well a child has learned a skill or understood a particular concept. It can also show how children have worked together during an activity, giving you an idea of how well they collaborate on tasks and problem-solving. Documentation includes

  • your notes and anecdotal records, photographs, recordings, transcripts, etc., regarding a child’s activities
  • samples of the child’s work that you have collected over time: drawings, paintings, photographs, collections, dictations, lists, models, and other representations of his or her knowledge and skills.

Assessment

“Assessment” refers to finding out what a child understands or knows how to do or how well she has learned a concept or skill. The documentation you collect can be useful with both formative and summative assessments of children’s progress in organizing, representing, and analyzing information.

Formative assessment tells you how a child is doing “in the moment.” What does he seem to understand about a topic or a process? What does he do that suggests that he is working on, mastering, or having trouble with a skill or concept?

Summative assessment helps you find out what a child knows, understands, or is able to do at the end of a particular time. How thorough and accurate is her understanding of the topic at the end of a project on tools people use? What skills does he have? What does she know now that she didn’t know at the beginning of the project?

This benchmark includes several points that the teacher can document and assess:

  • Can the child—with or without adult help—make a prediction about an event or activity, verbally or with a drawing, or both?
  • Can the child give reasons that supports her prediction, with or without adult help?
  • Can the child determine a way to “check” her prediction—that is, to compare it with the findings or outcome of the activity?

The table below provides examples of what a child might do at each level: exploring, developing, building.

EXPLORINGDEVELOPINGBUILDING
With teacher support, begin to predict the outcome of an activity (e.g., predict there are more boys than girls at the snack table).With teacher support, provide a reasonable prediction or guess for the outcome of an activity (e.g., predict that the class collected more yellow than red leaves on the nature walk before sorting and counting them).With teacher support, predict with more accuracy the outcome of a counting or comparison activity (e.g., predict how many more chairs, when three are already there, are needed for the small group table so that six children can all have a seat).

The following ideas may help you assess a child’s progress toward meeting this benchmark.

Keeping Documentation for Assessment

It’s a good idea to keep a documentation file for each child, with every item dated. Your documentation is a key to assessing the child’s ability to organize, represent, and/or analyze information.

A child’s file can include:

  • Notes on your observations relevant to his predictions—what he does and how he does it, whether he spontaneously makes predictions during play or conversations with peers and adults, how he explains his reasoning, etc.
  • Video and audio recordings (or transcripts of relevant portions) of the child involved in making predictions during class discussions, field work, or play. (For example of videos that document children making predictions, view the IEL Benchmark videos Magnets and Cars or Counting Chickens.)
  • Photographs of children during activities related to the benchmark. For additional insight, it’s a good idea to ask a child to tell you what is going on in a particular photograph.
  • Dated samples of the child’s sketches and drawings that express his predictions and plans. It’s a good idea to include notes about changes in the details and accuracy of his predictions over time.
Figure 11. The teacher might want to show this photograph to the child and ask her what she predicted – and what she found out – in response to the question about sounds

Assessing What the Child Can Do

Note whether the child seems able to imagine what is not “in front of her.” Specifically, is she able to talk about something that could happen in the future (for example, “later today,” “tomorrow,” or a more distant time)?

Note whether the child makes predictions during the day, outside the context of field work or class discussions. Examples might include:

  • “The red car will beat the green one on the ramp!”
  • “I need a short block to finish the wall. I’m doing long/short/long/short.”
  • “Teacher, where’s the tape? If I don’t have tape, my sign will fall over.”
  • “He’ll never let me use that toy.”
  • “Oh, that squirrel might take the corn!”
  • During pretend play: “I’ll give you a shot and you’ll get better, okay?”
  • “Hurry! We have to clean up in a minute!”

Note how a child makes a particular prediction.

  • Does she use a “wild guess,” an educated guess, or a fully formed hypothesis?
  • How does he respond when asked to explain a prediction (that is, when asked “What makes you think so?”)? What experiences or knowledge does he use as the basis for a prediction?
  • What does her prediction show about her understanding of number, sequences, patterns, story structure, probability, people’s feelings, or the topic involved?
  • Does he seem more confident about predictions in one area than in others? For example, can he make and support a relatively accurate prediction about how a character in a story might feel, but make a wild guess (“100!”) about how many blocks will result from combining five blocks and two blocks?

Compare a child’s later predictions with earlier samples to see how her understanding or skills have changed. This can be done as a formative assessment or a summative assessment.

Over time, observe the child’s approach to comparing his prediction to those made by his classmates, or to the information or data collected.

  • What is his response when the findings or outcome of an activity contradict his prediction? When they closely match it?
  • How does he respond to classmates’ predictions?
  • Do you notice changes over time?

Note how the child responds when asked to make a plan for something she wants to build or create. Can she name the thing she wants to make and state its purpose or what it will do? What details does she include (colors, relative sizes, etc.)? Can she anticipate what materials and equipment she will need? (For examples of how several children designed and made models of outdoor equipment based on their predictions about how water would behave with the materials they chose, see the IEL Benchmark videos Ideas about a Marble Run, Talking about Waterslide Models, and An Engineer Changes His Mind.)

Observe his predictions related to social-emotional understandings. Is he able to anticipate how a classmate or a book character might feel in a particular situation? Can he imagine his own possible feelings?

Sample Rubric for Child’s Predictions

Activity: Predict outcome of putting cup of milk in freezer and leaving it for four hours.

Evidence Base

Individual IELDS benchmarks may be derived from research or from what is generally understood to be the basis for good practice for teaching and learning. Learning benchmarks frequently are related to principles and standards developed by professional organizations concerned with the specific content area, such as those of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM), which are themselves based on both research and professional knowledge.

Professional Standards

Parallels to Benchmark 10.B.ECb can be found in the Head Start Child Outcomes Indicators. IELDS benchmark 10.B.ECb is also related to specific standards developed by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM) and to recommendations in a joint position statement of NCTM and the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC).

  • Head Start Child Outcomes Framework
    • Domain: Science, Scientific Skills & Methods: Participates in simple investigations to form hypotheses, gather observations, draw conclusions, and form generalizations. Describes and discusses predictions, explanations, and generalizations based on past experience.
  • National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM)
    • PreK–2 Expectations: In PreK through grade 2 all students should discuss events related to students’ experiences as likely or unlikely.
  • NAEYC/NCTM Joint Position Statement
    • Sample Teaching Strategies for Ages 3–6: Shows children the use of objects, fingers, counting on, guessing, and checking to solve problems.

Research

The following studies support the validity of involving preschool-age children in activities related to Benchmark 10.B.EC.b:


Atance, C. M., Louw, A, & Clayton, N. S. (2015). Thinking ahead about where something is needed: New insights about episodic foresight in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 129, 98–109. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.001

  • The authors discuss findings about the effects of temporal distance on future-oriented problem-solving in 3-, 4-, and 5-year olds. Responses of the 3-year-olds differed significantly from those of the 4- and 5-year-olds.

Atance, C. M., & Sommerville, J. A. (2014). Assessing the role of memory in preschoolers’ performance on episodic foresight tasks. Memory, 22, 118–128. doi:10.1080/09658211.2013.820324

  • In a study of 48 preschool children, the authors found cognitive development–related differences in children’s ability to choose an item that could solve a problem they had encountered earlier and in their memory for the problem.

Atance, C. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). My future self: Young children’s ability to anticipate and explain future states. Cognitive Development, 20, 341–361. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.05.001

  • The authors presented preschool-age children with stories and pictures meant to evoke their thinking about potential future states such as thirst, cold, and hunger. Children were asked to select one of three items they would need under each circumstance. Differences were found between the responses of 5-year-olds and those of 3- and 4-year-olds.

Bryant, P., & Nunes, T. (2012). Children’s understanding of probability: A literature review (Summary report). London, England: Nuffield Foundation.

  • The authors review contemporary research on what young children understand about probability, covering four subtopics: randomness and its consequences, understanding and analyzing the sample space, quantifying probability, and correlations.

Hanson, L. K., Atance, C. M., & Paluck, S. W. (2014). Is thinking about the future related to theory of mind and executive function? Not in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 120–137. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.07.006

  • Findings from this study of ninety 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children suggest that, in preschoolers, episodic foresight may not be related to their developing theories of mind or their executive function.

Kushnir, T., Xu, F. & Wellman, H. M. (2010). Young children use statistical sampling to infer the preferences of other people. Psychological Science, 21, 1134–1140. doi:10.1177/0956797610376652

  • The authors report findings of an experiment that suggest that preschoolers and 20-month-olds use a form of statistical information (violation of random sampling) to infer that another person prefers a particular type of toy over another type.

Kushnir, T. & Gopnik, A. (2007). Conditional probability versus spatial contiguity in causal learning: Preschoolers use new contingency evidence to overcome prior spatial assumptions. Developmental Psychology, 43, 186–196. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.186

  • In an experiment involving electronic toys that activated under certain conditions, the authors found that 3- and 4-year-olds were able to make accurate inferences about what caused the toys to activate, using new evidence in the form of patterns of probability.

Redshaw, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2013). Foresight beyond the very next event: Four-year-olds can link past and deferred future episodes. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 404. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00404

  • The authors found that 4-year-olds could solve a “deferred future problem” above the level of chance by selecting items that could help them solve a problem they encountered in one room after a 15-minute distraction in another room.

Russell, J., Cheke, L. G.., Clayton, N. S., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). What can what–when–where (WWW) binding tasks tell us about young children’s episodic foresight? Theory and two experiments. Cognitive Development, 26, 356–370. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.09.00

  • The authors examine three theoretical positions related to foresight and discuss two studies dealing with young children’s reasoning about events that might occur.

Zur, O., & Gelman, R. (2004). Young children can add and subtract by predicting and checking. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 121–137. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.003

  • The authors report on three experiments that indicate that 3- and 4-year-olds tended to propose reasonable cardinal values during the prediction phase and to use counting as the means of checking their predictions.

Professional Development Background and Procedures for This Module

The following standards and credentialing requirements may be addressed in this module.

Illinois Professional Teaching Standards

The following is a list of Illinois Professional Teaching Standards that may be addressed by completing the Benchmark 10.B.ECb MELBA (for use by instructors and students in teacher education programs).

  • 1I—stimulates prior knowledge and links new ideas to already familiar ideas and experiences
  • 1J—differentiates strategies, materials, pace, levels of complexity, and language to introduce concepts and principles so that they are meaningful to students at varying levels of development and to students with diverse learning needs
  • 3A—understands the Illinois Learning Standards, curriculum development process, content, learning theory, assessment, and student development and knows how to incorporate this knowledge in planning differentiated instruction
  • 3C—understands cultural, linguistic, cognitive, physical, and social and emotional differences, and considers the needs of each student when planning instruction
  • 3G—understands how research and data guide instructional planning, delivery, and adaptation
  • 3K—incorporates experiences into instructional practices that relate to a student’s current life experiences and to future life experiences
  • 6I—knows appropriate and varied instructional approaches, including those that develop word knowledge, vocabulary, comprehension, fluency, and strategy use in the content areas
  • 7B—understands that assessment is a means of evaluating how students learn and what they know and are able to do in order to meet the Illinois Learning Standards

Web Resources

PDF Printer-friendly PDF